Yoga Put to the Test for Depression, Anxiety, and Urinary Incontinence

4.5/5 - (52 votes)

Which of the 50 different yoga styles have been shown to be best?

Discuss
Republish

Below is an approximation of this video’s audio content. To see any graphs, charts, graphics, images, and quotes to which Dr. Greger may be referring, watch the above video.

Should doctors recommend their patients do yoga? As you’ve seen in my last few videos, the diculties associated with recommending yoga stem from the low quality of the scientific evidence available regarding its eects. Oddly, this lack of evidence is partly due to a common failure among researchers to define what they actually studied. They just say yoga, without defining what they mean. And different types of yoga dier greatly in what they demand in terms of physical strength, depth of meditation, breathing control, and spiritual component. Yet it’s very common for reports of studies not to define which type they used. This not only makes it hard to compare results between studies, it also makes it very hard to translate any findings to the bedside when counseling patients.

Yoga is broadly defined as a mind-body practice composed of physical postures, breathing techniques, and meditation. And so, if researchers say they put yoga to the test, you’d assume all three components would be present. However, some studies use meditation only, and call it yoga, with no postures or breathing. Others only examine breathing exercises, and call it yoga. Others consider yoga only postures, and still others say they’re studying forms of yoga without any of the three components. And so, if a study shows no benefit, does that really mean yoga, as commonly practiced, didn’t help? And, if a study does show benefit, what exactly do you tell patients to do to achieve it?

We can take some comfort in the fact that a review entitled “Is one yoga style better than another?” attempted to compare the effects of the more than 50 different yoga styles used in hundreds of trials, and they concluded that there was no apparent winner in terms of whether the results turned out positive. So, to a certain extent they may be interchangeable.

I’ve talked about the need to have active controls to see if there’s some benefit to yoga beyond just the exercise component. But such controls may still not deal with confounders, such as socialization. Yoga usually involves going to classes. So, you can imagine if you were studying the effects of yoga on something like depression; does just breaking routine, getting out, and meeting other people have any eect independent of the yoga?

Sadly, for depression, yoga doesn’t even seem to have a benefit ,compared to doing nothing. Yoga does appear to be helpful with anxiety, but not for people anxious to the extent that they’re actually diagnosed with an anxiety disorder. And, all the anxiety benefit from yoga appears just to be from the physical activity, since there appears to be no benefit compared to other types of exercise. So unfortunately, yoga does not appear to be effective for mood and anxiety disorders.

A review on the effects of yoga on depressive symptoms in people with mental disorders in general, including conditions like PTSD or schizophrenia, found no significant effect compared to things like social support that offer the same kind of attention, or to doing nothing, but did find an effect compared to so-called “waitlist” controls.

See, unlike in studies of drug effects, where a placebo sugar pill is relatively straightforward, selection of a control condition is much more complex for behavioral interventions like yoga, and one common solution is a passive control such as a waitlist. Basically, people are randomized to either yoga or onto a waitlist where they’ll still get yoga, but have to wait a while. Now, that’s better than having no control group at all, since it still controls for the passage of time––meaning whether or not your condition would have gotten better naturally on its own––but doesn’t really control for the placebo effect. If, instead, you were just told you were entering a study to see if some exercises may help, and then unbeknownst to you, randomized to do yoga versus some sort of sham yoga, then, you would have the same kind of expectation either way. But if you know going into it you’re going to get yoga now, which may help, or yoga in six months, and you end up in the waitlist control, this may spuriously amplify the difference in treatment effect, because people assigned to the waitlist may expect to not get better without active treatment.

For example, here’s a study for yoga on urinary incontinence. Women were randomized to a six-weeks-of-yoga or a waitlist control group. After six weeks, the incontinence frequency decreased by 70 percent in the yoga therapy, versus only 13 percent fewer episodes in the control group. But it’s possible that the women in the yoga group reported some benefits due to what’s called an expectation bias, because they expected yoga to be helpful. So, based on the body of evidence, one might say there’s insufficient good-quality evidence to judge whether yoga is useful for women with urinary incontinence. What we’d like to see are studies comparing yoga to time-and-attention control interventions––some alternative to yoga that entails the same amount of time and attention––so people don’t even know they’re in a control group.

Now, the question arises: who cares if the benefits from a yoga intervention are due to the placebo effect or expectation bias? A benefit is a benefit. As long as people get better, who cares? But it would be nice to know if yoga really helps or not. And here we go. Women with urinary incontinence were randomly assigned to a yoga group or a rigorous time-and-attention control group involving nonspecific muscle stretching and strengthening exercises.

There are two different types of urinary incontinence: urgency incontinence, defined as an involuntary loss of urine associated with a sudden strong desire to urinate, or stress incontinence, where an activity such as sneezing triggers an involuntary leak. Three months of yoga didn’t seem to significantly help overall incontinence, but it did seem to help with stress incontinence, decreasing by an average of 61 percent in the yoga group vs. only 35 percent in controls with no significant changes in urgency incontinence.

Please consider volunteering to help out on the site.

Motion graphics by Avo Media

Below is an approximation of this video’s audio content. To see any graphs, charts, graphics, images, and quotes to which Dr. Greger may be referring, watch the above video.

Should doctors recommend their patients do yoga? As you’ve seen in my last few videos, the diculties associated with recommending yoga stem from the low quality of the scientific evidence available regarding its eects. Oddly, this lack of evidence is partly due to a common failure among researchers to define what they actually studied. They just say yoga, without defining what they mean. And different types of yoga dier greatly in what they demand in terms of physical strength, depth of meditation, breathing control, and spiritual component. Yet it’s very common for reports of studies not to define which type they used. This not only makes it hard to compare results between studies, it also makes it very hard to translate any findings to the bedside when counseling patients.

Yoga is broadly defined as a mind-body practice composed of physical postures, breathing techniques, and meditation. And so, if researchers say they put yoga to the test, you’d assume all three components would be present. However, some studies use meditation only, and call it yoga, with no postures or breathing. Others only examine breathing exercises, and call it yoga. Others consider yoga only postures, and still others say they’re studying forms of yoga without any of the three components. And so, if a study shows no benefit, does that really mean yoga, as commonly practiced, didn’t help? And, if a study does show benefit, what exactly do you tell patients to do to achieve it?

We can take some comfort in the fact that a review entitled “Is one yoga style better than another?” attempted to compare the effects of the more than 50 different yoga styles used in hundreds of trials, and they concluded that there was no apparent winner in terms of whether the results turned out positive. So, to a certain extent they may be interchangeable.

I’ve talked about the need to have active controls to see if there’s some benefit to yoga beyond just the exercise component. But such controls may still not deal with confounders, such as socialization. Yoga usually involves going to classes. So, you can imagine if you were studying the effects of yoga on something like depression; does just breaking routine, getting out, and meeting other people have any eect independent of the yoga?

Sadly, for depression, yoga doesn’t even seem to have a benefit ,compared to doing nothing. Yoga does appear to be helpful with anxiety, but not for people anxious to the extent that they’re actually diagnosed with an anxiety disorder. And, all the anxiety benefit from yoga appears just to be from the physical activity, since there appears to be no benefit compared to other types of exercise. So unfortunately, yoga does not appear to be effective for mood and anxiety disorders.

A review on the effects of yoga on depressive symptoms in people with mental disorders in general, including conditions like PTSD or schizophrenia, found no significant effect compared to things like social support that offer the same kind of attention, or to doing nothing, but did find an effect compared to so-called “waitlist” controls.

See, unlike in studies of drug effects, where a placebo sugar pill is relatively straightforward, selection of a control condition is much more complex for behavioral interventions like yoga, and one common solution is a passive control such as a waitlist. Basically, people are randomized to either yoga or onto a waitlist where they’ll still get yoga, but have to wait a while. Now, that’s better than having no control group at all, since it still controls for the passage of time––meaning whether or not your condition would have gotten better naturally on its own––but doesn’t really control for the placebo effect. If, instead, you were just told you were entering a study to see if some exercises may help, and then unbeknownst to you, randomized to do yoga versus some sort of sham yoga, then, you would have the same kind of expectation either way. But if you know going into it you’re going to get yoga now, which may help, or yoga in six months, and you end up in the waitlist control, this may spuriously amplify the difference in treatment effect, because people assigned to the waitlist may expect to not get better without active treatment.

For example, here’s a study for yoga on urinary incontinence. Women were randomized to a six-weeks-of-yoga or a waitlist control group. After six weeks, the incontinence frequency decreased by 70 percent in the yoga therapy, versus only 13 percent fewer episodes in the control group. But it’s possible that the women in the yoga group reported some benefits due to what’s called an expectation bias, because they expected yoga to be helpful. So, based on the body of evidence, one might say there’s insufficient good-quality evidence to judge whether yoga is useful for women with urinary incontinence. What we’d like to see are studies comparing yoga to time-and-attention control interventions––some alternative to yoga that entails the same amount of time and attention––so people don’t even know they’re in a control group.

Now, the question arises: who cares if the benefits from a yoga intervention are due to the placebo effect or expectation bias? A benefit is a benefit. As long as people get better, who cares? But it would be nice to know if yoga really helps or not. And here we go. Women with urinary incontinence were randomly assigned to a yoga group or a rigorous time-and-attention control group involving nonspecific muscle stretching and strengthening exercises.

There are two different types of urinary incontinence: urgency incontinence, defined as an involuntary loss of urine associated with a sudden strong desire to urinate, or stress incontinence, where an activity such as sneezing triggers an involuntary leak. Three months of yoga didn’t seem to significantly help overall incontinence, but it did seem to help with stress incontinence, decreasing by an average of 61 percent in the yoga group vs. only 35 percent in controls with no significant changes in urgency incontinence.

Please consider volunteering to help out on the site.

Motion graphics by Avo Media

Doctor's Note

This is the fourth video in my six-part series on yoga. If you missed any of the first three, see: 

The final two videos are coming up: Yoga Put to the Test for IBS, Inflammatory Bowel, Menopause, and Osteoporosis and The Side Effects of Yoga.

If you haven't yet, you can subscribe to our free newsletter. With your subscription, you'll also get notifications for just-released blogs and videos. Check out our information page about our translated resources.

Subscribe to our free newsletter and receive the Purple Sweet Potato Longevity Smoothie recipe from How Not to Age.

Pin It on Pinterest

Share This